In
March, 2019, a new antidepressant became available in the United
States. This has been classed as "the
first fundamentally new medicine for depression in decades".
The controversial thing is that it's based on the anesthetic,
ketamine. (The drug itself is called Spravato.) Some psychiatrists
have said that this drug was approved with little evidence to back it
up (see later comments); and also under standards that were less
rigorous than those required for “conventional” antidepressants.
Experiments
on Mice
Mice
were the guinea pigs (as it were) in one bit of research on
ketamine. The researchers induced depression/stress in mice by
restraining them in mesh tubes or by injecting them with
corticosterone
(a stress hormone). It was then seen that the mice began to loose
their taste for sweet water and didn't struggle when dangled by their
tails. (All this raises animal welfare issues; which are too complex and tangential to go into here.)
It
can be asked how the scientists came to know that the
depression/stress of these mice had lifted. Did the mice express that
view? Of course not. So it must have been behavioural evidence that convinced those involved in this research.
Well,
three hours after a dose of ketamine, the mice’s behaviour did indeed improve (at least in these cases).
Of
course no one can really make a big deal about ketamine making mice
(or people) feel better “within hours”. (Typical antidepressants
can take weeks to start working.) In terms of fast action, alcohol and cannabis can make people feel better within minutes. LSD and
“magic mushrooms” can do the same thing within around 30 minutes
to an hour. Indeed I suppose that a heavy smash on the head could (in
theory) do the same within seconds.
It
was thought (at first) that ketamine “spurred”
new synapses (i.e., the connections between neurons). In turned out
that newly created synapses aren't actually involved in ketamine's behavioural and mood effects. Nonetheless, synapses are still part of
the story of ketamine and its effects.
This
shows us that we should never take a single study as gospel, or even
a whole group of studies. We - laypersons and scientists alike -
often need to sit back and take stock.
For
example, who remembers when depression was seen to be all about
serotonin?
A few weeks later (to be rhetorical), it was seen to be all about
dopamine or cat piss. Thus any research into depression (or indeed
into anything science-based) should be taken with extreme care.
That's not a “anti-science” statement. It's simply a statement
about particular “research projects” (or “studies”) at
particular points in time. Indeed being slightly sceptical about the
glut of often contradictory scientific findings is often a highly
scientific and rational position to take. If one were to accept all
of them all of the time, then one would be rendered highly confused and,
indeed, irrational.
This
also means that one shouldn't be committed to particular scientists,
particular studies, or even particular disciplines within science.
Instead one should be committed to the communal reality of science as
a whole; which usually has a self-correcting bias.
The
physicist Lee Smolin (in his The
Trouble With Physics) sums part of this up neatly in
the following passage:
“Scientists
often do exaggerate and distort the evidence. Age, status, fashion,
peer pressure all do play a role in the workings of the scientific
community... But I would suggest that enough scientists adhere to
enough of the ethic [i.e., the scientific ethic] that in the long run
progress continues to be made...”
The
anti-depression industry is huge. So we must also factor in
financial clout and political pressure/power into these studies, as
well as the importance of the U.S. Food
and Drug Administration. I mention the the FDA
because it approved a nasal spray (as stated in the intro) which
contains a form of ketamine (i.e., esketamine)
for depression only in March, 2019.
Technical
Terms Galore
Of
course there are lots of technical terms to cope with in these
studies. For example, we're told that ketamine slowly coaxes
nerve cells in order to sprout new neural connections. More
technically, some 12 hours after ketamine treatment, new dendritic
spines began to appear in the neurons found in a part of
the prefrontal cortex of mice. These dendritic spines have been seen
to to be replacing those lost during the period of stress or
depression.
Prima
facie, there's an assumption here that new neural connections are automatically a good thing. So what if a paranoid schizophrenic is
making new neuronal connections which all re-enforce his paranoia? And isn't it the case that new neural connections are being
made all the time in animal brains?
In
any case, the neuroscientist Alex
Kwan (of Yale University) happily
tells us that there “is still
a lot of mystery in terms of how ketamine works in the brain”.
But isn't that the case with so many drugs which effect the brain and
therefore the mind – even those which have been on the market for
decades?
Having
put certain negatives, there will be a temptation to criticise the
positives simply because it's ketamine that we're talking about here. Yes, ketamine is not only a drug – it can also be an illegal drug
(i.e., it's a “controlled
substance”). Though, as everyone knows, the various social, moral and legal lines between
illegal and legal drugs are often vague, highly contingent and very
rarely backed up with good arguments.
No comments:
Post a Comment